Little kids, big insights: What childhood can teach us about how the brain supports cognition
or technically,
The age of reason: Functional brain network development during childhood
[See Original Abstract on Pubmed]
Authors of the study: Ursula A. Tooley, Anne T. Park, Julia A. Leonard, Austin L. Boroshok, Cassidy L. McDermott, Matthew A. Tisdall, Dani S. Bassett, and Allyson P. Mackey.
Early and middle childhood (4-10 years old) are full of developmental milestones. How children speak, move, learn, and play is constantly evolving and improving. Kids build social networks, become better able to control their attention, and begin to develop cognitive skills, like reasoning. However, despite the rapid cognitive development happening during this early childhood period, neuroscientists have very little information about how brain function is changing.
This is because getting a clear picture of brain activity, like getting a clear picture of anything, requires that the subject stays almost perfectly still. If you’ve ever watched a 4-year-old sit at the dinner table, it comes as no surprise that they don’t make the best neuroimaging subjects. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scanners, which take many consecutive snapshots of brain activity, are even more sensitive to motion than cameras. The tiniest movements, even just a few millimeters, can blur the images and make it impossible for neuroscientists to tell what brain activity belongs to which brain region. So, most neuroimaging work to date uses subjects over 7 years old, which means that while researchers work to understand how the brain develops to support cognition, they’re missing many of the first pieces of the puzzle.
Here’s where recent Neuroscience Graduate Group alumn Ursula Tooley and collaborators from the Robust Methods for Magnetic Resonance group stepped in. The team engineered a way to monitor and correct for head motion inside the scanner, allowing them to collect high-quality neuroimaging data from wiggly subjects during this critical early childhood period. Specifically, this motion-tracking technology gave the researchers a way to record exactly how much and in which direction kids were moving at any given point during the scan. Ursula could then use this information to correct (think: realign) the images of brain activity or exclude the child from the study if they moved too much. The ability to precisely monitor head position in real time also created an opportunity for kids to practice the correct behavior. Before the scanning session, children came to the lab to watch a movie while laying in a mock scanner that made the same whirring noises and beeps as the real deal. Each time they moved their head more than 1 millimeter, the movie paused. Incorporating this period of exploring the scanner and the scanning expectations meant that most of the kids who enrolled in the study stayed still enough for usable images of brain activity to be collected. This is a huge feat for Ursula and the team as well as a huge win for neuroscience, making it possible to take an earlier look at the developing brain.
Over the course of the study, Ursula and her colleagues scanned a diverse group of 92 children ages 4-10 from the Philadelphia community. Each child completed an fMRI scan as well as a series of cognitive tests (which they did outside of the scanner) designed to measure the strength of their cognitive reasoning abilities. What is cognitive reasoning? Reasoning is an umbrella term describing the ability to process information, problem solve, and make predictions based on pattern recognition (Fig. 1). Successful cognitive reasoning involves much of the brain and improves dramatically during early and middle childhood. Research suggests that how kids perform on cognitive reasoning tasks is predictive of their academic achievement — even years down the road! By combining a child's cognitive reasoning ability with information about their brain activity, Ursula was able to ask whether and how changes in brain function might support this shift in cognitive performance.
Ursula used resting-state fMRI data (data collected while the kids laid “at rest” in the scanner) to explore the brain’s functional organization. In other words, she inferred how much different brain regions talk to each other based on how their activity fluctuates together over time. As such, regions with activity that rises and falls together are likely functionally connected. These groups of connected brain regions are called “systems.” The brain has many of these functionally-connected systems, and neuroscience research shows that they can rewire and reconfigure themselves. For example, another neuroimaging study of older kids and young adults (ages 8-22) from Philadelphia showed that the organization of these brain systems changes with age [1]. Specifically, our brain systems become more segregated and more modular as we move towards adulthood, with weaker connections between systems and stronger connections within systems (Fig. 2). Ursula found the same trends in her data with older kids tending to have more segregated brain systems than younger kids, suggesting that our brain’s functional architecture is flexible and continues to refine as we age.
Do some systems remodel more than others? Ursula found that changes in connectivity were largest in brain systems involved in abstract cognition, visual processing, and attention. As it turns out, these are the same systems involved in cognitive reasoning. For instance, reasoning is supported by the brain’s visual areas taking in information from the world while attention systems focus the brain’s resources on what’s important to the task at hand while ignoring distractors. Given what we know about the blossoming of cognitive reasoning during childhood, Ursula wondered if there could be a relationship between these changes in brain connectivity and cognitive ability. To test this, Ursula compared the patterns of brain system connectivity for each child with their scores on the cognitive reasoning test (Fig. 1). She found that the remodeling of cognition, visual processing, and attention systems was associated with increased cognitive ability! In other words, kids who had more mature patterns of brain system connectivity were better equipped to reason about the world and their place in it.
Taken together, Ursula’s work suggests that the massive restructuring of brain systems as kids age might be happening to support the rapid development of cognitive abilities emerging during these early and middle childhood years. Beyond offering a new perspective on healthy brain development, this relationship between brain organization and brain function offers new ways to think about -- and potentially treat -- various neurodevelopmental or neurological disorders.
Citations:
Baum, G.L., Ciric R., Roalf, D.R., Betzel, R.F., Moore, T.M., Shinohara, R.T., … & Satterthwaite, T.D. (2017). Modular segregation of structural brain networks supports the development of executive function in youth. Current Biology, 27(11). doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.04.051.